This article was originally published with errors, which have now been corrected in the online version. Correction https://doi.org/10.15304/rge.34.1.10482.
1. INTRODUCTION
Over the past two decades, researchers have increasingly recognized personality as a crucial motivator of human behavior (; ). defined personality as “the set of psychological traits and mechanisms within the individual that are organized and relatively enduring, and that influence his or her interactions with, and adaptations to the environments (including the intrapsychic, physical, and social environments)”. Initially studied within social psychology, this concept has since spread to the field of behavioral finance ().
Social science has long sought to identify factors contributing to an individual’s financial well-being. However, neither demographic nor economic variables can explain the significant cross-sectional variations of this state (). Understanding the sources of individual variation in financial behavior and outcomes is crucial for effective behavioral and policy interventions to be devised that can be aimed at enhancing financial well-being.
Household finance, an essential area of study, involves decisions regarding savings and financing. described this term as the practices of what financial instruments and markets family units utilize to achieve their goals and aspirations. Despite its growing significance (; ), the relationship between personality traits and household finance remains underexplored. Given the importance of household decisions in the modern economy, it is vital to understand how these choices are made.
Three notable studies have investigated the relationship between personality traits and household finance. examined the influence of the former on financial decisions related to unsecured debts and financial assets in the UK. explored the relationship between these human characteristics and financial distress among young adults in the US. More recently, investigated the impact of retail borrowers' personality traits on their ability to manage debt repayment difficulties by way of an extensive online survey conducted in the UK. All three studies identified significant associations between this mindset and financial outcomes. However, they did not address the relationship between personality traits and three crucial dimensions of household finance: 1) the decision to hold secured debts, 2) the likelihood of experiencing financial distress, and 3) the likelihood of achieving financial well-being.
This study aims to fill the void in the literature by examining the impact of the former on the latter three dimensions in 31 European countries, utilizing the EU-SILC dataset from Eurostat. The choice of geography is motivated by the cultural diversity of these countries, as identified by Hofstede's (, ) cultural dimensions.
Our study has several features which deserve to be highlighted. To begin with, to the best of our knowledge, this article is the first to examine the impact of personality traits on household financial decisions in Europe, the analysis of which spans 31 countries. Specifically, we have analyzed how neuroticism, extraversion, and agreeableness influence three dimensions of household finance: the decision to hold secured debts, and the likelihood of experiencing financial distress and achieving financial well-being. Understanding these financial dimensions in Europe is particularly important given the region's significant role in the global economy. For example, in 2022, the national debt in the 27 European Union countries amounted to €50.58 trillion (), compared to the US national debt of $30.93 trillion ().
Secondly, as far as we are aware, this is the first investigation to employ the EU-SILC dataset from Eurostat for this purpose. The use of this dataset enables comprehensive cross-national comparisons in a culturally diverse region, offering valuable insights into household financial behavior in Europe.
Our findings provide the first insights into the impact of individual personality traits on key dimensions of household finance in the European context. Specifically, our evidence reveals that neuroticism and extraversion are negatively associated with the likelihood of holding a mortgage, while agreeableness is positively associated with mortgage amounts. Neuroticism and extraversion correlate positively with the likelihood of experiencing financial distress, whereas agreeableness correlates negatively. Furthermore, neurotic and extraverted individuals are less likely to achieve financial well-being, while agreeable individuals are more likely to do so.
The paper is structured as follows: section 2 reviews related literature and develops the hypotheses to be tested; section 3 describes the data and methodology; section 4 presents the empirical results; section 5 discusses these results; lastly, section 6 concludes the paper.
2. LITERATURE REVIEW
2.1. Personality traits and household finance
The literature on behavioral finance has highlighted several factors influencing individuals' decision-making processes, including personality traits and demographic characteristics. Households often need to face challenging decisions under uncertain conditions. According to prospect theory, psychological factors play a significant role in causing deviations from perfectly rational decision-making (), a phenomenon that can be further exacerbated by one’s misunderstanding of one’s own risk perception (; ).
Personality traits refer to the set of perceptual, cognitive, emotional, and motivational systems in an individual that affect decision-making responses to environmental stimuli (). Various efforts have been made to measure personality traits. For instance, the Myers-Briggs Type Indicator (MBTI) framework categorizes personality into four dichotomies: 1) judging versus perceiving, 2) sensing versus intuition, 3) thinking versus feeling, and 4) extraversion versus introversion. Established by Carl Jung in 1921, the MBTI considers personality as a preference in decision-making rather than a fixed characteristic ().
Building on the MBTI, several personality models have been developed. introduced the 16 Personality Factors (16PF) model, which includes five global factors and 16 primary traits. developed the widely-used Big Five personality traits framework, classifying personality into extraversion, openness, agreeableness, conscientiousness, and neuroticism. These traits have been extensively utilized in psychology and increasingly in economics ().
emphasized the significance of household finance, now a distinct field of research (). Households use financial instruments throughout their lives, including cash, checks, and credit cards for purchasing goods and services, and they invest in durable goods and human capital to fund present and future consumption. They require liquidity to manage risks beyond their control, such as those of health and property. Therefore, knowledge about payment choices, debt financing, savings, and insurance contracts is essential ().
Several studies have examined the impact of personality traits on savings and borrowing behavior. Using a Dutch dataset, found that agreeableness and neuroticism are positively associated with the likelihood of holding a mortgage, while extraversion is negatively associated with the mortgage amount. Other studies have explored the influence of personality on unsecured debts and assets. , using the British Household Panel Survey (BHPS), found that extraversion (positively) and conscientiousness (negatively) are associated with the probability of amassing a credit card debt. They also found that extraversion is negatively related to the likelihood of holding shares. However, found that personality traits are weak predictors of debts such as those of credit cards, contrary to and . More recently, , utilizing a survey regarding the Chilean economy, concluded that conscientiousness positively predicts pension and bank savings behavior.
The literature has revealed that personality traits can significantly predict economic behavior, particularly in money management skills, and financial decision-making (; ). discovered that neuroticism is negatively correlated with money management skills, while found significant impacts of personality traits on financial decision-making in the Australasian market. Additionally, showed that extraversion and neuroticism influence financial behaviors in an experimental asset market, with extraverted individuals paying higher prices and buying more when assets are overpriced, and neurotic individuals holding fewer risky assets in their portfolios.
Research has also examined the effects of personality traits on the likelihood of experiencing financial distress. found that low self-reports of neuroticism during adolescence predict less economic pressure in adulthood, while concluded that neurotic and agreeable individuals are more likely to need financial assistance. discovered that neuroticism and agreeableness are positively correlated with the likelihood of financial distress among young adults in the US, while conscientiousness and extraversion are negatively associated with financial distress.
Conversely, other studies have investigated the influence of personality traits on the likelihood of achieving financial and social well-being. found that agreeableness and extraversion (positively) and neuroticism (negatively) correlate with social well-being. However, concluded that there is no significant relationship between personality traits and financial well-being. More recently, examined the relationship between personality traits and the subjective well-being of young adults. They came to the conclusion that extraversion, conscientiousness, and agreeableness were positively associated with subjective well-being, while neuroticism was negatively associated with it.
In summary, previous studies have suggested that personality traits can significantly predict various financial dimensions, such as savings and borrowing behavior (), money management (), and financial behaviors in the asset market (). However, there are a lack of studies examining the relationship between personality traits and household finance in Europe. Therefore, this study aims to investigate the influence of personality traits on three household finance dimensions in the continent: 1) the decision to hold secured debts, 2) the likelihood of experiencing financial distress, and 3) the likelihood of achieving financial well-being.
2.2. Development of hypotheses
The hypotheses developed in this section are grounded in three key theoretical foundations: the first one is that individual personality traits exist and are stable enough to be identified (); the second states that these traits can be categorized using the Big Five personality traits framework. Finally, these personality traits play a significant role in shaping financial decisions at the household level (; ; ).
model includes extraversion, openness, agreeableness, conscientiousness, and neuroticism. Each trait is measured in terms of six specific facets. Table 1 presents the definitions of these personality traits and their respective facets.
Source: and
This study focuses on three of the Big Five personality traits: neuroticism, agreeableness, and extraversion. Due to a lack of data in the EU-SILC for openness and conscientiousness, it is not feasible to explore all of the personality traits. Previous research has examined the relationship between certain ones and the decision to hold different types of debts. For instance, found that levels of agreeableness and neuroticism were positively associated with the likelihood of holding a mortgage, whereas extraversion was negatively associated with the mortgage amount. noticed a positive association between extraversion and the probability of running up credit card debts. Neurotic individuals tend to be emotional when having to face financial challenges, which can impair their ability to think clearly, make decisions, and cope with stress, leading to irrational actions (). Thus, we hypothesize that neurotic individuals are more likely to accrue debt. In contrast, extraverted individuals are more likely to experience positive emotions and avoid distress (), leading us to predict that extraverted individuals will be less likely to incur debts. Agreeable individuals, being more influenced by others and more doubtful of their decisions (), are expected to be more likely to amass debts.
Based on these findings, we propose the following three hypotheses regarding the decision to hold secured debts:
-
Hypothesis 1: The level of agreeableness in an individual is positively related to the likelihood of their household incurring a debt.
-
Hypothesis 2: The level of neuroticism in an individual is positively related to the likelihood of their household incurring a debt.
-
Hypothesis 3: The level of extraversion in an individual is negatively related to the likelihood of their household incurring a debt.
Research has also suggested that certain personality traits are associated with financial distress. found that low levels of neuroticism during adolescence predict less economic pressure in adulthood. showed that neuroticism and agreeableness are positively correlated with the need for financial assistance. concluded that neuroticism and agreeableness are positively associated with financial distress. Additionally, found that neuroticism is negatively correlated with money management skills, while demonstrated that conscientiousness and extraversion are negatively associated with financial distress among young adults in the United States. Borrowers who show neurotic tendencies are found to be more likely to undertake high-risk strategies when faced with unforeseen issues that affect their ability to keep up with their debt interest and repayments (). Extraverted individuals tend to seek instant gratification, spend more, and save less, which can increase their likelihood of financial distress (). Conversely, neurotic individuals are prone to excessive worry and psychological distress (), increasing their likelihood of financial distress.
Based on these findings, we propose the following hypotheses regarding financial distress:
-
Hypothesis 4: The level of agreeableness in an individual is positively related to the likelihood of their household being in a state of financial distress.
-
Hypothesis 5: The level of neuroticism in an individual is positively related to the likelihood of their household being in a state of financial distress.
-
Hypothesis 6: The level of extraversion in an individual is positively related to the likelihood of their household being in a state of financial distress.
Several studies have examined the relationship between personality traits and financial or social well-being. found that agreeableness and extraversion levels are positively and negatively correlated with social well-being, respectively. have reported that extraversion and agreeableness are positively associated with subjective well-being, while neuroticism is negatively associated with it. However, concluded that there is no significant association between personality traits and financial well-being. Neurotic individuals are more likely to experience excessive anger and psychological distress (), leading us to hypothesize that neurotic individuals are less likely to achieve financial well-being. Similarly, extraverted individuals' tendency to seek instant gratification and spend more makes them less likely to achieve financial well-being (). Additionally, the indecisiveness and lack of confidence in the decisions of agreeable individuals () suggest a lower likelihood of reaching a state of financial well-being.
Based on this reasoning, we have formulated the following three hypotheses regarding this state:
-
Hypothesis 7: The level of agreeableness in an individual is negatively related to the likelihood of his/her household experiencing financial well-being.
-
Hypothesis 8: The level of neuroticism in an individual is negatively related to the likelihood of his/her household experiencing financial well-being.
-
Hypothesis 9: The level of extraversion in an individual is negatively related to the likelihood of his/her household experiencing financial well-being.
To control for individual demographic characteristics, such as gender, age, income, health, education, marital status, activity status, tenure status, and country of residence, we have followed the existing literature (; ; ; ). Moreover, we have included the squared age to account for the non-linear effects of individuals' personalities throughout their lifetimes ().
3. DATA AND METHODOLOGY
3.1. Data
To examine the relationship between personality traits and household finance, we utilized data from the EU-SILC dataset, obtained from the Eurostat Database (see Appendix 1 for Supplementary Data). The EU-SILC is a comprehensive tool designed to collect both cross-sectional and longitudinal microdata on income, poverty, social exclusion, and living conditions. It covers both objective and subjective aspects of these topics in monetary and non-monetary terms for households and individuals. Specifically, the data provides information on personality traits as of 2013.
The data collection was performed choosing from these five modes: 1) pen-and-paper personal interviewing (PAPI), 2) computer-assisted personal interviewing (CAPI), 3) computer-assisted telephone interviewing (CATI), 4) self-administered survey, or 5) proxy interview. The dataset includes information on individuals' personality traits, demographic characteristics, and household finance dimensions at the household level. The household respondent for the finance dimensions was selected based on the following priorities: 1) the main person responsible for the upkeep of the dwelling, and 2) a household member aged 16 or over who would be best placed to provide this information.
The sample consisted of individuals aged 16 and above from 31 European countries, including 26 European Union (EU) member states and the rest outside the EU (Iceland, Norway, Serbia, Switzerland, and the UK). The average number of observations per country was 16,194.
This study investigates individuals' personality traits based on the Big Five Personality Traits framework (). Due to the lack of data on openness and conscientiousness in the EU-SILC, we focused on neuroticism, agreeableness, and extraversion as the main independent variables. Neuroticism was measured using a scale based on , , and . Agreeableness was analyzed with a scale based on , and for extraversion, one was used based on .
The study examines three dimensions of household finance: 1) the decision to hold secured debts, based on Brown et al. (, ), and , 2) the likelihood of being in a state of financial distress, based on Xu et al. (, ), and 3) the likelihood of being in a state of financial well-being, based on , and . These three dimensions are considered to be our dependent variables. We have controlled for the most important individual demographic characteristics: age, squared age, gender, marital status, level of education, basic activity status, total household income, general health, tenure status, and country of residence. The measurements of all these variables are shown in Appendix 2, "Definitions of Variables".
Summary statistics for the dependent, independent, and control variables are shown in Table 2.
Note: The data is cross-sectional at the individual and household level from the EU-SILC dataset. We standardized total household income using Z-score standardization. The logs of total mortgage and total household income are expressed in Euros. Age is expressed in years. Refer to Appendix 2 for the definitions of the variables.
The average age of the population in our sample was 56.50 years. Notably, 25% of households held mortgages, and 59% could afford a one-week annual holiday.
3.2. Methodology
For our analysis, we employed logistic regression for binary dependent variables, which were as follows: i) holding a mortgage, ii) the ability to afford a one-week holiday, and iii) facing unexpected financial expenses. For the ordinal dependent variables, we used ordered logistic regression, which included: i) arrears on hire purchase instalments or other loan payments, ii) arrears on mortgage or rental payments, iii) arrears on bills, iv) the ability to make ends meet, v) the burden of total housing costs, and vi) the burden of repaying debts. Additionally, we used generalized linear models with a Gaussian distribution for the log of the total mortgage amount.
To mitigate multicollinearity issues, we individually entered the personality traits into the regressions. We began with a baseline regression controlling for age, squared age, gender, and country. Furthermore, we conducted additional estimates controlling for other individual demographic characteristics.
The following three main regression models were used to test our research hypotheses:
where p = P( Holding secured debts = 1).
The tenure status variable was excluded in this equation to avoid multicollinearity issues, as the “holding a mortgage” variable was derived from tenure status.
where p = P( The likelihood of being in a state of financial distress = 1)
where p = P( The likelihood of being in a state of financial well — being = 1).
4. EMPIRICAL RESULTS
4.1. The decision to hold secured debts
Table 3 presents the baseline logistic regression results for the decision to hold a mortgage, along with the generalized linear models (GLM) with a Gaussian distribution for the log of the total mortgage amount. Robust Standard Errors were employed in the GLM regression to address heteroscedasticity.
Dependent variables | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Holding a mortgage | Log of the total mortgage | ||||||||
(1) | (2) | (3) | (4) | (5) | (6) | ||||
Neuroticism | -0.141*** (0.006) | -0.044*** (0.008) | |||||||
Agreeableness | -0.002 (0.001) | 0.003** (0.001) | |||||||
Extraversion | -0.059*** (0.005) | -0.028*** (0.007) | |||||||
Gender [Male] | 0.050*** (0.009) | 0.058*** (0.008) | 0.072*** (0.009) | 0.048*** (0.011) | 0.029*** (0.009) | 0.053*** (0.011) | |||
Age | 0.153*** (0.002) | 0.133*** (0.001) | 0.150*** (0.002) | 0.056*** (0.002) | 0.055*** (0.002) | 0.056*** (0.002) | |||
Squared age | -0.002*** (0.000) | -0.001*** (0.000) | -0.002*** (0.000) | -0.001*** (0.000) | -0.001*** (0.000) | -0.001*** (0.000) | |||
Intercept | -3.774*** (0.056) | -3.532*** (0.044) | -3.877*** (0.056) | 6.224*** (0.067) | 6.158*** (0.050) | 6.196*** (0.068) | |||
Country | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | |||
Num. Obs. | 356,542 | 500,928 | 364,888 | 70,094 | 107,279 | 70,666 | |||
AIC | 293,694 | 422,216 | 297,876 | 248,980 | 383,884 | 251,312 | |||
BIC | 294,071 | 422,605 | 298,254 | 249,292 | 384,210 | 251,623 | |||
Log.Lik. | -146,812 | -211,073 | -148,903 | -124,456 | -191,908 | -125,622 |
Note: This table presents the results of the logistic regression and GLM models for the determinants of holding a mortgage (listed in the third column) and the log of the total mortgage (listed in the fourth column), respectively. The models include the following variables, listed in the first column: Neuroticism, Agreeableness, Extraversion, Gender, Age, Squared age, and Country. Standard errors are reported in parentheses. Statistical significance is indicated by asterisks:
Refer to Appendix 2 for the definitions of the variables.
In regressions (1 to 3), we observed a negative association between the likelihood of holding a mortgage and the levels of two personality traits: neuroticism and extraversion. These results were statistically significant at the 1% level. Additionally, the data indicated that males and older individuals were more likely to hold a mortgage, with statistical significance at the 1% level.
In regressions (4 to 6), the levels of neuroticism and extraversion were also found to be negatively associated with the mortgage amount, again with statistical significance at the 1% level. Conversely, the level of agreeableness was shown to be positively associated with the mortgage amount, with significance at the 1% level. Furthermore, males and older individuals were seen as being more likely to have a higher mortgage amount (significance at the 1% level). These findings contradict , who claimed that these personality traits are not significant predictors of debt, but they do support the results of .
Additional estimates controlling for other demographic characteristics such as income, health, education, marital status, activity status, and tenure status (not reported) reaffirmed that neuroticism and extraversion levels were negatively associated with the likelihood of holding a mortgage (significance at the 1% level). The level of agreeableness remained positively associated with both the likelihood of holding a mortgage and the mortgage amount, with statistical significance at the 1% level. However, the statistical significance of neuroticism was slight when controlling for additional demographics.
In summary, hypotheses (1) and (3) regarding the effects of extraversion and agreeableness on the decision to hold secured debt should not be rejected. However, hypothesis (2), positing a positive impact of neuroticism, can be ruled out, as our results indicated a negative impact of neuroticism on the decision to hold secured debt.
4.2. The likelihood of being in a state of financial distress
Table 4 presents the baseline logistic regression results for the determinants of the likelihood of being in a state of financial distress. We controlled for age, squared age, gender, and country. Ordered logistic regressions were employed for the following five dependent variables: 1) the financial burden of the total housing cost, 2) the financial burden of the repayment of debts from hire purchases or loans, 3) arrears on bills, 4) arrears on hire purchase instalments or other loan payments, and 5) arrears on mortgage or rental payments.
Dependent variables | ||||||||||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Burden of THC | Burden of repayment debts | Arrears on bills | Arrears on HPI/LP | Arrears on M/RP | ||||||||||||
(1) | (2) | (3) | (4) | (5) | (6) | (7) | (8) | (9) | (10) | (11) | (12) | (13) | (14) | (15) | ||
Neuroticism | 0.554*** (0.005) | 0.576*** (0.009) | 0.641*** (0.007) | 0.636*** (0.012) | 0.686*** (0.012) | |||||||||||
Agreeableness | -0.042*** (0.001) | -0.042*** (0.002) | -0.051*** (0.002) | -0.052*** (0.003) | -0.056*** (0.003) | |||||||||||
Extraversion | 0.088*** (0.003) | 0.110*** (0.007) | 0.046*** (0.004) | 0.060*** (0.009) | 0.062*** (0.009) | |||||||||||
Gender [Male] | -0.073*** (0.007) | -0.117*** (0.006) | -0.146*** (0.007) | -0.035*** (0.013) | -0.078*** (0.011) | -0.117*** (0.013) | -0.011 (0.011) | -0.053*** (0.009) | -0.066*** (0.011) | 0.046** (0.021) | -0.028 (0.017) | -0.015 (0.020) | 0.064*** (0.021) | -0.037** (0.017) | -0.033* (0.020) | |
Age | -0.207*** (0.023) | -0.036*** (0.018) | -0.042* (0.022) | 0.066 (0.050) | 0.216*** (0.037) | 0.271*** (0.049) | 0.223*** (0.038) | 0.478*** (0.030) | 0.523*** (0.037) | 0.011 (0.076) | 0.388*** (0.060) | 0.375*** (0.074) | 0.167** (0.077) | 0.420*** (0.060) | 0.463*** (0.075) | |
Squared Age | 0.016 (0.022) | -0.126*** (0.018) | -0.125*** (0.022) | -0.244*** (0.052) | -0.350*** (0.040) | -0.423*** (0.051) | -0.702*** (0.038) | -0.860*** (0.031) | -0.936*** (0.037) | -0.338*** (0.082) | -0.623*** (0.065) | -0.637*** (0.079) | -0.596*** (0.084) | -0.757*** (0.067) | -0.822*** (0.081) | |
Country | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | |
Num. Obs. | 356,200 | 499,954 | 364,489 | 89,106 | 133,093 | 90,466 | 354,007 | 497,164 | 362,249 | 100,461 | 149,155 | 102,111 | 148,991 | 216,511 | 151,361 | |
AIC | 622,971 | 890,067 | 652,256 | 159,705 | 244,667 | 166,356 | 266,165 | 383,390 | 283,209 | 76,378 | 111,813 | 80,759 | 84,359 | 122,945 | 89,241 | |
BIC | 623359 | 890,467 | 652,645 | 160,043 | 245,019 | 166,695 | 266,553 | 383,790 | 283,597 | 76,721 | 112,170 | 81,102 | 84,716 | 123,315 | 89,599 | |
Log.Lik. | -311,449 | -444,997 | -326,092 | -79,816 | -122,297 | -83,142 | -133,046 | -191,659 | -141,568 | -38,153 | -55,870 | -40,343 | -42,143 | -61,436 | -44,584 |
Note: This table presents the results of ordered logistic regressions for the determinants of the financial burden of the total housing cost (listed in the third column), the financial burden of the repayment of debts from hire purchases or loans (listed in the fourth column), arrears on bills (listed in the fifth column), arrears on hire purchase instalments or other loan payments (listed in the sixth column), and arrears on mortgage or rental payments (listed in the seventh column), respectively. The models include the following variables listed in the first column: Neuroticism, Agreeableness, Extraversion, Gender, Age, Squared age, and Country. Standard errors are reported in parentheses. Statistical significance is indicated by asterisks
Refer to Appendix 2 for the definitions of the variables.
From regressions 1 to 15 in Table 4, we observed that the levels of neuroticism and extraversion were positively associated with all five measures for the likelihood of financial distress (significance at the 1% level). Conversely, agreeableness was negatively associated with these measures, with the findings being statistically significant at the 1% level. Our results concerning neuroticism were consistent with those of , whereas for agreeableness and extraversion, there was a notable difference, the former observing a positive association with financial distress for their study, the latter, a negative one, whereas our results indicated the opposite.
To further explore the determinants for the likelihood of financial distress, we conducted additional analyses controlling for demographic variables such as income, health, education, marital status, activity status, and tenure status (not reported). The positive associations of neuroticism and extraversion and the negative one of agreeableness with financial distress were the same under these controls at the statistical significance level of 1%.
Our findings also indicated that the higher the levels of income, education and health were, the lower the likelihood of financial distress. Age, however, appeared to increase it, with older individuals being more susceptible. Gender differences were also noted: males were more prone to arrears on debts like utility bills, mortgages or rental payments, and hire purchase instalments, but were less likely to be burdened by total housing costs and loan repayments. Marital status influenced financial distress, with married or never-married individuals being less likely to experience it. Employment status also played a role, as those who were working or retired faced less financial distress compared to the unemployed. Additionally, individuals who fully owned their properties or received free housing were less likely to be in financial distress. All these inferences were based on results that were significant at conventional levels of statistical significance.
In conclusion, our results consistently rejected hypothesis (4), confirming that agreeableness was negatively associated with financial distress. However, we could not reject hypotheses (5) and (6) concerning the influences of neuroticism and extraversion on the likelihood of financial distress.
4.3. The likelihood of being in a state of financial well-being
In this section, we examine the relationship between personality traits and the probability of attaining financial well-being. Table 5 presents the outcomes of the basic logistic regressions for the determinants of the probability of reaching a state of financial well-being, considering age, age squared, gender, and country. We employed logit models for two dependent variables: the ability to afford a one-week yearly vacation and the capacity to cope with unforeseen financial expenses. Additionally, we used an ordered logit model for the dependent variable: the ability to make ends meet.
Dependent Variables | |||||||||
---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|---|
Ability to afford vacation | Unexpected FE | Ability to make ends meet | |||||||
(1) | (2) | (3) | (4) | (5) | (6) | (7) | (8) | (9) | |
Neuroticism | -0.633*** (0.005) | -0.622*** (0.005) | -0.696*** (0.004) | ||||||
Agreeableness | 0.056*** (0.001) | 0.040*** (0.001) | 0.048*** (0.001) | ||||||
Extraversion | -0.105*** (0.003) | -0.087*** (0.003) | -0.105*** (0.003) | ||||||
Gender [Male] | 0.101*** (0.008) | 0.120*** (0.006) | 0.178*** (0.007) | 0.161*** (0.008) | 0.163*** (0.006) | 0.235*** (0.007) | 0.067*** (0.006) | 0.119*** (0.005) | 0.157*** (0.006) |
Age | 0.054*** (0.001) | 0.038*** (0.001) | 0.040*** (0.001) | 0.046*** (0.001) | 0.023*** (0.001) | 0.033*** (0.001) | 0.452*** (0.021) | 0.206*** (0.016) | 0.243*** (0.020) |
Squared age | 0.000*** (0.000) | 0.000*** (0.000) | 0.000*** (0.000) | 0.000*** (0.000) | 0.000*** (0.000) | 0.000*** (0.000) | -0.288*** (0.020) | -0.087*** (0.016) | -0.111*** (0.020) |
Intercept | 1.467*** (0.048) | 0.260*** (0.039) | 0.679*** (0.046) | 1.247*** (0.046) | 0.353*** (0.037) | 0.428*** (0.044) | |||
Country | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes | Yes |
Num. Obs. | 356,248 | 499,905 | 364,560 | 356,746 | 500,785 | 365,049 | 356,930 | 501,101 | 365,233 |
AIC | 401,196 | 569,951 | 427,795 | 421,052 | 604,721 | 448,767 | 1,031,876 | 1,485,543 | 1,083,465 |
BIC | 401,574 | 570,341 | 428,173 | 421,430 | 605,110 | 449,145 | 1,032,296 | 1,485,977 | 1,083,886 |
Log.Lik. | -200,563 | -284,940 | -213,862 | -210,491 | -302,325 | -224,348 | -515,899 | 742,732 | -541,693 |
Notes: This table presents the results of logistic regression and ordered logistic regression models for the determinants of the ability to afford a one-week yearly vacation, the capacity to cope with unforeseen financial expenses (listed in the third and fourth columns), and the ability to make ends meet (listed in the fifth column), respectively. The models include the following variables listed in the first column: Neuroticism, Agreeableness, Extraversion, Gender, Age, Squared age, and Country. Standard errors are reported in parentheses. Statistical significance is indicated by asterisks:
Refer to Appendix 2 for the definitions of variables.
The regression results (1 to 9) presented in Table 5 indicate that neuroticism and extraversion were negatively associated with all three measures of financial well-being, while agreeableness was positively associated with these measures. These associations were statistically significant at the 1% level. Our finding regarding agreeableness aligned with , who identified a positive correlation between personality traits and social well-being. In contrast, our results contradicted , whose study found no significant relationship between personality traits and financial well-being but did observe a positive link between money management skills and wealth accumulation.
Further analysis (not reported here) entailed controlling for income, health, education, marital status, activity status, and tenure status, and re-confirmed our main conclusions regarding personality traits and financial well-being.
Overall, we continued to reject hypothesis (7), confirming that agreeableness was positively associated with financial well-being. However, we could not reject hypotheses (8) and (9) regarding the impact of neuroticism and extraversion on financial well-being.
5. DISCUSSION
Our findings on the influence of personality traits on mortgage decisions aligned with . Specifically, extraversion was negatively associated with mortgage amounts, whereas agreeableness was positively associated with the likelihood of holding a mortgage. These results indicate that neuroticism and extraversion negatively influenced the decision to hold a mortgage, while agreeableness had a positive effect. This stood in contrast to , who reported that extraversion, agreeableness, and neuroticism were not significant predictors of debt. However, they identified a positive association between conscientiousness and debt and a negative one between openness and debt.
The discrepancies between our findings and those of may have stemmed from differences in sample size and sampling methodology. Our study utilized a representative probability sample of 502,017 individuals and households from 31 European countries, which likely provided more statistically significant results compared to convenience sample of 993 students in the United States.
Our results also suggested that neuroticism was positively associated with financial distress, consistent with . However, our evidence indicated that agreeableness negatively impacted financial distress, while extraversion positively affected it. These findings contradicted , who reported the opposite signs for agreeableness and extraversion coefficients. The discrepancy may have stemmed from differences in the scope of financial distress measurements and sample demographics. Our study used five financial distress measures compared to the two used by . Additionally, our sample included individuals aged 23 to 88 from 31 European countries, while focused exclusively on young adults aged 24 to 34 in the United States.
Moreover, our results implied that agreeableness positively affected financial well-being, consistent with , while neuroticism and extraversion negatively impacted it. This contrasted with , who found no significant relationship between personality traits and financial well-being. The discrepancy may have arisen from use of a convenience sample of 355 individuals in the United States, which may not have been representative of other cultural contexts. Additionally, their study collected data on the Internet, possibly targeting a younger, better-educated demographic. Furthermore, focused on the impact of money management skills on financial well-being, rather than the direct effects of personality traits.
Although our paper has not focused on the impact of national cultures in all of their manifestations, for the dimensions of household finance examined in our study, it would be wise to discuss the potential interaction between cultural variables and the individual personality traits captured by the variables in our analysis. According to the literature, there is evidence suggesting that Hofstede's (, ) cultural dimensions correlate with the Big Five personality traits identified by and (; ; ). For instance, reported a correlation coefficient of 0.64 between the cultural dimension of individualism and the personality trait of extraversion, a statistically significant relationship at the 0.1% level.
In the context of our study, in theory, the findings linking higher levels of extraversion with a greater likelihood of households experiencing financial distress and a higher probability of holding a mortgage could be replicated if we replaced the explanatory variable representing extraversion with one reflecting the cultural dimension of individualism as defined by Hofstede (, ). However, it is not prudent to overemphasize the importance of cultural factors, as the literature has suggested that the explanatory power of cultural variables in cross-country studies may be weakening, attributed to cultural globalization, which is significantly eroding the distinct cultural traits of individual countries ().
6. CONCLUSIONS
This study has advanced the literature on household finance by exploring the determinants of key financial dimensions in 31 European countries. Specifically, we analyzed how personality traits influenced three critical aspects of household finance: the decision to hold secured debts, the likelihood of experiencing financial distress, and the likelihood of achieving financial well-being. To our knowledge, this is the first study to have utilized the EU-SILC dataset from Eurostat for this purpose.
Our findings indicated that personality traits significantly impacted household finance decisions and outcomes. Notably, individuals with high neuroticism and extraversion were less likely to hold a mortgage and, when they did, tended to take on larger mortgage amounts. Conversely, individuals with high agreeableness were more likely to hold a mortgage and incur higher mortgage repayments. Additionally, neuroticism and extraversion were positively correlated with financial distress, whereas agreeableness showed a negative correlation. Nonetheless, neurotic and extraverted individuals were less likely to be in a positive state of financial well-being, whereas agreeable individuals were more likely to achieve it. These associations remained significant even after controlling for various demographic variables.
Our study has highlighted the importance of considering personality traits when evaluating financial decisions and outcomes. It has suggested that behavioral interventions tailored to personality differences could be beneficial. To mitigate the negative effects of financial risk, we recommend that governments and financial institutions design financial products and services that cater to different personality profiles, by helping households to save, invest, and manage mortgages more effectively. Financial regulators should establish guidelines so that financial institutions can enhance transparency and raise awareness of the influence of personality traits on financial behavior and offer financial planning programs to address adverse cases of it.
Furthermore, individuals should be cognizant of how their personality traits affect their financial choices and outcomes. This self-awareness could facilitate more informed decision-making that would positively impact financial well-being. However, caution must be exercised regarding their ability to accurately assess their own personality traits and, consequently, their capacity to independently extract the lessons contained in our study. We advocate for public policies that promote financial literacy by way of educational programs and social media outreach. Our research offers valuable insights for policymakers aiming to improve household welfare and contributes to the academic understanding of the factors influencing household finance decisions.
Given our findings, specifically the positive association between the personality traits of neuroticism and extraversion and the likelihood for individuals to encounter financial distress, the policy recommendations outlined here should prioritize those exhibiting both of these traits. Such a focus would be particularly important during periods of significant economic downturns or recessions, as these are times when financial distress is likely to reach socially alarming levels.
Like most academic studies, our research has certain limitations that warrant acknowledgment. For instance, due to the constraints of the EU-SILC dataset, the impact of the personality traits of openness and conscientiousness on the three dimensions of household finance examined in this study could not be analyzed. This limitation is particularly noteworthy, as existing literature has established that, for example, the trait of conscientiousness plays a critical role in predicting pension and bank savings (). Additionally, the dataset's design constrains the personality trait measures available in the dataset.
As future research directions, we suggest that scholars consider incorporating variables capable of capturing the influence of various dimensions of national cultures on financial decisions made within households. Furthermore, it would be worthwhile to address the limitations of the dataset used in this study by enabling the inclusion of personality traits not examined here, such as extraversion and openness to experience. Finally, it would be interesting to investigate the potential mediating role that variables such as financial literacy or risk preferences, among others, may play in terms of the effect of personality traits on households' financial decisions.
Acknowledgments
Centro de Economia e Finanças (CEF.UP) is financed by Portuguese public funds through FCT - Fundação para a Ciência e Tecnologia, I.P.
Author contributions
Conceptualization, J. L. and R.I. A.; Methodology, J. L. and R.I. A.; Software, R.I. A.; Validation, J. L. and R.I. A.; Formal Analysis, J. L. and R.I. A.; Data Curation, J. L. and R.I. A.; Writing - Original Draft Preparation, J. L. and R.I. A.; Writing - Review & Editing, J. L. and R.I. A. All authors have read and agreed to the published version of the manuscript.
REFERENCES
1
Allik, J. (2005). Personality dimensions across cultures. Journal of Personality Disorders, 19, 212–232. https://doi.org/10.1521/pedi.2005.19.3.212
2
Almlund, M., Duckworth, A.L., Heckman, J., & Kautz, T. (2011). Personality Psychology and Economics. In Handbook of the Economics of Education (Vol. 4, pp. 1–181). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-444-53444-6.00001-8
3
Álvarez-Espiño, M., Fernández-López, S., & Rey-Ares, L. (2023). Profile of financially fragile households: Comparative analysis of Galicia and Spain. Revista Galega de Economía, 32(1), 1-36. https://doi.org/10.15304/rge.32.1.8437
4
Brown, S., Garino, G., & Taylor, K. (2008). Mortgages and Financial Expectations: a Household-level Analysis. Southern Economic Journal, 74(3), 857–878. https://www.jstor.org/stable/20112000%0A
5
Brown, S., Garino, G., & Taylor, K. (2013). Household Debt and Attitudes Toward Risk. Review of Income and Wealth, 59(2), 283–304. https://ideas.repec.org/a/bla/revinw/v59y2013i2p283-304.html
6
Brown, S., & Taylor, K. (2008). Household Debt and Financial Assets: Evidence from Germany, Great Britain and the USA. Journal of the Royal Statistical Society, 171(3), 615–643. https://www.jstor.org/stable/30135088%0A
7
Brown, S., & Taylor, K. (2014). Household Finances and the ‘Big Five’Personality Traits. Journal of Economic Psychology, 45, 197–212. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2014.10.006
8
Campbell, J.Y. (2006). Household Finance. Journal of Finance, 61(4), 1553–1604. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1540-6261.2006.00883.x
9
10
Costa Jr, P.T., McCrae, R.R., & Kay, G.G. (1995). Persons, Places, and Personality: Career Assessment Using the Revised NEO Personality Inventory. Journal of Career Assessment, 3(2), 123–139. https://doi.org/10.1177/106907279500300202
11
Costa, P.T., & McCrae, R.R. (1992). Normal Personality Assessment in Clinical Practice: The NEO Personality Inventory. Psychological Assessment, 4(1), 5. https://doi.org/10.1037/1040-3590.4.1.5
12
13
Donnellan, M.B., Conger, K.J., McAdams, K.K., & Neppl, T.K.. (2009). Personal Characteristics and Resilience to Economic Hardship and Its Consequences: Conceptual Issues and Empirical Illustrations. Journal of Personality, 77(6), 1645–1676. https://doi.org/10.1111/j.1467-6494.2009.00596.x
14
Donnelly, G., Iyer, R., & Howell, R.T. (2012). The Big Five Personality Traits, Material Values, and Financial Well-being of Self-described Money Managers. Journal of Economic Psychology, 33(6), 1129–1142. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2012.08.001
15
Durand, R.B., Newby, R., & Sanghani, J. (2008). An Intimate Portrait of the Individual Investor. Journal of Behavioral Finance, 9(4), 193–208. https://doi.org/10.1080/15427560802341020
16
Erjavec, J., Popovič, A., & Trkman, O. (2019). The Effect of Personality Traits and Knowledge on the Quality of Decisions in Supply Chains. Economic Research-Ekonomska Istraživanja, 32(1), 2269–2292. https://doi.org/10.1080/1331677X.2019.1642788
17
Eurostat. (2023). Government debt. https://ec.europa.eu/eurostat/databrowser/view/GOV_10Q_GGDEBT__custom_4412334/default/table?lang=en
18
Fan, L., Chatterjee, S., & Kim, J. (2022). Young Adults’ Personality Traits and Subjective Well-being: The Role of Perceived Money Management Capability. Journal of Behavioral and Experimental Finance, 35, 100689. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jbef.2022.100689
19
Garðarsdóttir, R.B., & Dittmar, H. (2012). The Relationship of Materialism to Debt and Financial Well-being: The Case of Iceland’s Perceived Prosperity. Journal of Economic Psychology, 33(3), 471–481. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2011.12.008
20
Gillen, M., & Kim, H. (2014). Older Adults’ Receipt of Financial Help: Does Personality Matter? Journal of Family and Economic Issues, 35(2), 178–189. https://doi.org/10.1007/s10834-013-9365-0
21
Guiso, L., & Sodini, P. (2013). Household Finance: An emerging field. In Handbook of the Economics of Finance (Vol. 2, pp. 1397–1532). Elsevier. https://doi.org/10.1016/B978-0-44-459406-8.00021-4
22
Hill, P.L., Turiano, N.A., & Roberts, B.W. (2012). Examining Concurrent and Longitudinal Relations between Personality Traits and Social Well-being in Adulthood. Social Psychological and Personality Science, 3(6), 698–705. https://doi.org/10.1177/1948550611433888
23
Hirsh, J.B. (2015). Extraverted Populations Have Lower Savings Rates. Personality and Individual Differences, 81, 162–168. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2014.08.020
24
25
Hofstede, G. (2011). Dimensionalizing Cultures: The Hofstede Model in Context. Online Readings in Psychology and Culture, 2(1), 919–2307. https://doi.org/10.9707/2307-0919.1014
26
Hofstede, G., & McCrae, R.R. (2004). Personality and culture revisited: Linking traits and dimensions of culture. Cross-Cultural Research, 38(1), 52–88. https://doi.org/10.1177/1069397103259443
27
Johnston, D.W., Kassenboehmer, S.X., & Shields, M.A. (2016). Financial Decision-Making in the Household: Exploring the Importance of Survey Respondent, Health, Cognitive Ability and Personality. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 132, 42–61. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2016.09.014
28
Kausel, E., Hansen, E., & Tapia, P. (2016). Responsible Personal Finance: The Role of Conscientiousness in Bank and Pension Savings in Chile. International Review of Finance, 16(1), 161-167. https://doi.org/10.1111/irfi.12069
29
Kumar, P., Islam, M.A., Pillai, R., & Tabash, M.I. (2024). Risk Perception-Perceived Investor Performance Nexus: Evaluating the Mediating Effects of Heuristics and Prospects With Gender as a Moderator. SAGE Open, 14(2), 21582440241256444. https://doi.org/10.1177/21582440241256444
31
Leary, M.M., Reilly, M.D., & Brown, F.W. (2009). A Study of Personality Preferences and Emotional Intelligence. Leadership & Organization Development Journal, 30(5), 421–434. https://doi.org/10.1108/01437730910968697
32
Lobão, J. (2024). The Influence of Gender on Individuals’ Ability to Predict Their Own Risk Tolerance: Evidence from a European Country. Administrative Sciences, 14(3), 56. https://doi.org/10.3390/admsci14030056
33
Nyhus, E.K., & Webley, P. (2001). The Role of Personality in Household Saving and Borrowing Behaviour. European Journal of Personality, 15(S1), S85–S103. https://doi.org/10.1002/per.422
34
Oehler, A., & Horn, M. (2020). Behavioural Portfolio Theory Revisited: Lessons Learned from the Field. Accounting & Finance, 61, 1743–1774. https://doi.org/10.1111/acfi.12643
35
Oehler, A., Wendt, S., Wedlich, F., & Horn, N. (2018). Investors’ Personality Influences Investment Decisions: Experimental Evidence on Extraversion and Neuroticism. Journal of Behavioral Finance, 19(1), 30–48. https://doi.org/10.1080/15427560.2017.1366495
36
Philippas, N.D., & Avdoulas, C. (2020). Financial Literacy and Financial Well-being among Generation-Z University Students: Evidence from Greece. European Journal of Finance, 26(4–5), 360–381. https://doi.org/10.1080/1351847X.2019.1701512
37
Rendal, S., Brooks, C., & Hillenbrand, C. (2021). The Impacts of Emotions and Personality on Borrowers’ Abilities to Manage Their Debts. International Review of Financial Analysis, 74, 101703. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.irfa.2021.101703
38
Rizvi, S., & Fatima, A. (2015). Behavioral Finance: A Study of Correlation Between Personality Traits with the Investment Patterns in the Stock Market. In Managing in Recovering Markets (pp. 143–155). Springer. https://doi.org/10.1007/978-81-322-1979-8_11
39
Stackhouse, M., Rickley, M., Liu, Y., & Taras, V. (2024). Homogeneity, heterogeneity, or independence? A multilevel exploration of Big Five personality traits and cultural values in 40 nations. Personality and Individual Differences, 230, 112795. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.paid.2024.112795
40
Stagner, R. (1948). Psychology of Personality. McGraw-Hill. https://psycnet.apa.org/record/1948-04844-000
41
Statista. (2023). National Debt. https://www.statista.com/statistics/187867/public-debt-of-the-united-states-since-1990/
42
Triandis, H.C., & Suh, E.M. (2002). Cultural influences on personality. Annual Review of Psychology, 53, 133–160. https://doi.org/10.1146/annurev.psych.53.100901.135200
43
Tversky, A., & Kahneman, D. (1979). Prospect Theory: An Analysis of Decision under Risk. Econometrica, 47(2), 263–291. https://doi.org/10.2307/1914185
44
Xu, Y., Beller, A.H., Roberts, B.W., & Brown, J.R. (2015). Personality and Young Adult Financial Distress. Journal of Economic Psychology, 51, 90–100. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.joep.2015.08.010
45
Xu, Y., Briley, D.A., Brown, J.R., & Roberts, B.W. (2017). Genetic and Environmental Influences on Household Financial Distress. Journal of Economic Behavior & Organization, 142, 404–424. https://doi.org/10.1016/j.jebo.2017.08.001
46
Zehra, Nahid, & Singh, U.B. (2023). Household Finance: a Systematic Literature Review and Directions for Future Research. Qualitative Research in Financial Markets, 15(5), 841-887. https://doi.org/10.1108/QRFM-11-2021-0186
Appendix
Appendix 1. Supplementary data
Supplementary data to this article can be found online under official request to the Eurostat database at https://ec.europa.eu/eurostat/web/microdata/european-union-statistics-on-income-and-living-conditions.